Saturday, October 5, 2024

An AI Podcast - About a Nerdy Law Review Article (Slocum & Tobia 2023)

 



The new thing is how Google can auto-generate AI podcasts, with two chipper and eager AI voices discussing whatever PDF’s you’ve uploaded into a Google Notebook LM folder.

Here, I tried to bore the two voices to death with an obscure Harvard Law Review article.  Slocum & Tobia 2023:

https://harvardlawreview.org/forum/vol-137/the-linguistic-and-substantive-canons/

The Linguistic and Substantive Canons...

###

Here is a link to the streaming, two-voice AI podcast.   (Open access but may require Google sign in).

https://notebooklm.google.com/notebook/ad3b6f8d-bd29-44a6-a810-c36c9bcbd972/audio

##

And below is an TRANSCRIPT of the AI PODCAST about SLOCUM.

 

##

 

Brenda:

Hey, there. Ready to unlock some secret legal code.

Adam:

Ooh, secret legal code. I like the sound of that.

Brenda:

We're talking about linguistic and substantive canons.

Adam:

Okay.

Brenda:

Those interpretive tools courts use to decipher what laws actually mean.

Adam:

Right.

Brenda:

Think of this deep dive as your crash course in legal linguistics without the law school tuition.

Adam:

Yeah, it's true. These canons are often seen as the hidden machinery of the legal system, shaping how laws are understood and applied. We're diving into a fascinating academic response from the Harvard Law Review Forum, which offers a fresh take on these interpretive tools.

Brenda:

Okay. Let's unpack this.

Adam:

Okay.

Brenda:

So traditionally, we've got these two camps, linguistic canons and substantive canons.

Adam:

Right.

Brenda:

It's the classic battle between language nerds and policy wonks.

Adam:

Exactly. Linguistic canons are all about understanding the literal meaning of words, like dictionaries and grammar rules.

Brenda:

Right.

Adam:

On the other hand, substantive canons are more like policy safeguards, often used to rein in overly-broad interpretations of a law.

Brenda:

So one group is focused on the what, the actual words, while the other is more interested in the why, the intention behind them.

Adam:

That's a great way to put it. But what's so intriguing about this response article is that it argues these canons aren't playing on opposite teams. Instead, they exist on a spectrum with some canons having both linguistic and substantive weight.

Brenda:

Interesting.

Adam:

It's not an either situation.

Brenda:

So it's not just about deciphering the words themselves, but also understanding the unspoken rules about how we use language to create rules in the first place.

Adam:

Precisely. Take the presumption against retroactivity, for example. This legal principle states that new laws shouldn't apply to things that happen before they existed unless it explicitly says so.

Brenda:

Okay.

Adam:

Typically, we view this through a lens of fairness, a substantive concern. Punishing someone for something that wasn't against the rules at the time feels unjust. Right?

Brenda:

Totally.

Adam:

It'd be like getting detention for wearing a band T-shirt to school last week, even though the rule against it wasn't announced until today.

Brenda:

Exactly. But here's where it gets interesting. This response suggests that our aversion to retroactivity might also stem from how we naturally understand and interpret rules, even outside of a legal context. It's not just about fairness, but about an intuitive grasp of language and how rules work in the real world.

Adam:

So it's like when your teacher says, "No phones during class," there's this unspoken understanding that it applies from that moment forward, not to every time you've ever used your phone in class before.

Brenda:

That's a fantastic analogy. And to test this idea, the authors actually conducted empirical studies observing how everyday people interpret rules in various scenarios to see if those interpretations align with these traditional canons. They took legal theory out of the courtroom and into the real world.

Adam:

Now, that's what I'm talking about, putting our legal common sense to the test. So what did they find? Did our everyday understanding of rules hold up?

Brenda:

Well, they discovered some pretty compelling evidence to support their theory. They designed some clever experiments, presenting people with various scenarios involving rules, some legal, some more everyday situations, like business agreements or even sports rules.

Speaker 3:

[inaudible 00:03:16].

Brenda:

Okay. The key was to see how people reacted when a rule was applied retroactively to something that happened *before* the rule was in place.

Adam:

So testing whether someone thinks it's fair to penalize a soccer player for a handball if the rule was established *after* the game was played.

Brenda:

Precisely. And when it came to the presumption against retroactivity, the results were pretty definitive. People overwhelmingly felt that it was wrong to punish someone for something that wasn't against the rules when they did it. It makes sense, doesn't it? We instinctively understand that rules should generally apply from the point they established and not before.

Adam:

Absolutely. Nobody likes a retroactive rule change, especially when there are consequences involved. It feels fundamentally unfair.

Brenda:

Exactly. Now with anti-extraterritoriality, the principle that a rule shouldn't apply outside its intended jurisdiction, things got a bit more nuanced. This is where the specific context of the rule really came into play.

Adam:

So like how a school rule against chewing gum probably doesn't apply to students when they're at home, even though technically they're still students of the school.

Brenda:

That's a great example. While people generally grasp the idea that rules have boundaries, where those boundaries lie often depended on the nature of the rule itself. For instance, a company rule might apply to all its employees, regardless of where they're working remotely, while a local traffic law obviously wouldn't apply to someone driving in a different country.

Adam:

It highlights how our intuitions about rules are shaped by our understanding of context and jurisdiction. Fascinating. What else did they discover?

Brenda:

Their experiments also shed light on the anti-implied repeal principle, which to put it simply means people generally assume a new law doesn't completely erase an older one unless it's explicitly stated.

Adam:

Right.

Brenda:

Think of it like adding a new amendment to the Constitution.

Adam:

Yeah.

Brenda:

It doesn't invalidate everything that came before it. It's about building upon and clarifying existing laws.

Adam:

Right. It's more like adding a new layer of interpretation rather than a complete overhaul.

Brenda:

Right, right.

Adam:

So we've seen how people actually think about these canons in practice, but the big question is what does this all mean? Why should we care if our gut feelings about rules align with these legal principles? This is where it gets really interesting. This research has major implications for how we understand legal interpretation, especially when it comes to textualism.

Brenda:

That's right. Textualism, as you might recall, is a legal philosophy that emphasizes the plain meaning of a legal text. Think of it as a very literal approach to interpreting laws, prioritizing dictionaries, grammar rules, and the like.

Adam:

So textualists are like the legal sticklers for the rules of language.

Brenda:

Exactly. They believe judges should focus on what the law actually *says*, not what lawmakers might have *meant* to say, which can sometimes feel at odds with common sense.

Adam:

I see. So how does this research showing that our common sense understanding of rules often aligns with the legal canons impact the textualist approach?

Brenda:

Well, traditionally, textualists have been wary of substantive canons, those policy-driven ones, because they seem too open to interpretation. It's like giving judges a free pass to inject their own values into the law instead of sticking to the text itself.

Adam:

Right. It's like saying, "Well, the law doesn't explicitly forbid me from wearing a chicken costume in court, so I guess it's fair game."

Brenda:

Precisely. But if, as this research suggests, some of these substantive canons are actually rooted in how we instinctively understand and use language, then they might not be so subjective after all. They could actually be considered part of a law's plain meaning, making them fair game, even for the most devout textualist.

Adam:

That's a really compelling point. So this isn't just an academic debate. It could actually shift how judges interpret laws in the real world with real-world consequences.

Brenda:

Absolutely. And this isn't just a hypothetical scenario. This very question is playing out right now in the debate surrounding the major questions doctrine.

Adam:

The major questions doctrine. Refresh my memory on that one.

Brenda:

Essentially, it's the idea that government agencies need extremely clear direction from Congress before taking major actions that impact the entire country. It's been used to challenge significant agency actions in recent years, with some arguing it gives judges too much power to overrule the decisions of regulatory bodies.

Adam:

So if this research is correct and the major questions doctrine is actually reflecting a deeper understanding of how we use language to delegate authority, it might hold more weight than some critics argue.

Brenda:

Exactly. It suggests that what might seem like a policy preference could actually be rooted in our shared linguistic understanding of how rules and authority function. This research throws a fascinating curveball into the ongoing debate.

Adam:

It definitely makes you rethink some assumptions. Taking a step back, what's the biggest takeaway for our listeners? What's the core message we should leave them with?

Brenda:

I think the key takeaway here is that the line between language and policy in the legal world might be blurrier than we previously thought. Linguistic and substantive canons, often viewed as separate entities, could be more intertwined than we realized. And as textualism continues to hold sway in our courts, this new perspective has the potential to reshape how laws are interpreted and applied.

Adam:

This deep dive has given us a lot to think about. It's fascinating to consider that there might be other linguistic decoder rings hidden within legal language, shaping our understanding of the law in ways we're just beginning to uncover.

So to our listeners, we leave you with this thought-provoking question. If our intuitive grasp of language plays such a significant role in legal interpretation, what does that say about the law's claim to objectivity and neutrality? It's a question that legal scholars will likely be grappling with for years to come. And if you're curious to delve further into this fascinating debate, we highly recommend checking out the full response article in the Harvard Law Review Forum. It's a thought-provoking read that will deepen your understanding of this complex topic.

Speaker 3:

I second that recommendation.

Adam:

Until next time, keep those legal brains buzzing.